The psychology of professional and student actors: Creativity, personality, and motivation

Denis Dumas, Conceptualization , Formal analysis , Methodology , Project administration , Software , Writing – original draft , Writing – review & editing , 1, * Michael Doherty, Conceptualization , Data curation , Project administration , Writing – review & editing , 2 and Peter Organisciak, Data curation , Formal analysis , Methodology , Software , Writing – review & editing 1

Denis Dumas

1 Department of Research Methods and Information Science, University of Denver, Denver, Colorado, United States of America

Find articles by Denis Dumas

Michael Doherty

2 Actor’s Equity Association, New York, NY, United States of America

Find articles by Michael Doherty

Peter Organisciak

1 Department of Research Methods and Information Science, University of Denver, Denver, Colorado, United States of America

Find articles by Peter Organisciak Paolo Roma, Editor

1 Department of Research Methods and Information Science, University of Denver, Denver, Colorado, United States of America

2 Actor’s Equity Association, New York, NY, United States of America Sapienza, University of Rome, ITALY

Competing Interests: Michael Doherty’s affiliation with Actor’s Equity Association did not and does not alter our adherence to PLOS ONE policies on sharing data and material.

Received 2020 Jan 27; Accepted 2020 Oct 1. Copyright © 2020 Dumas et al

This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.

Associated Data

Attachment: Submitted filename: renamed_594c2.docx GUID: 2E6CBB17-74E1-4478-8A0C-EC15AFB40382

Attachment: Submitted filename: manuscript review- May 2020 The Psychology of Professional and Student Actors.docx

GUID: F4D0E94B-3951-4AF1-8A07-474D0D3F29AE Attachment: Submitted filename: Revision_Review for Plus One.docx GUID: 0561FA41-D74B-457B-8077-03D1EDA214CA

Attachment: Submitted filename: manuscript- May 2020 The Psychology of Professional and Student Actors- RE-REVIEW.docx

GUID: CD544570-3E02-4344-BBC4-E025189ED67D

To facilitate replicability and open science, the data collected and analyzed in this study is archived at Zenodo (LINK: https://zenodo.org/record/3899579#.X3-M81KSlaQ; DOI: 10.5281/zenodo.3899578), and the text-mining based models used to score the divergent thinking tasks are freely available on our laboratory website (https://openscoring.du.edu/).

Abstract

As a profession, acting is marked by a high-level of economic and social riskiness concomitantly with the possibility for artistic satisfaction and/or public admiration. Current understanding of the psychological attributes that distinguish professional actors is incomplete. Here, we compare samples of professional actors (n = 104), undergraduate student actors (n = 100), and non-acting adults (n = 92) on 26 psychological dimensions and use machine-learning methods to classify participants based on these attributes. Nearly all of the attributes measured here displayed significant univariate mean differences across the three groups, with the strongest effect sizes being on Creative Activities, Openness, and Extraversion. A cross-validated Least Absolute Shrinkage and Selection Operator (LASSO) classification model was capable of identifying actors (either professional or student) from non-actors with a 92% accuracy and was able to sort professional from student actors with a 96% accuracy when age was included in the model, and a 68% accuracy with only psychological attributes included. In these LASSO models, actors in general were distinguished by high levels of Openness, Assertiveness, and Elaboration, but professional actors were specifically marked by high levels of Originality, Volatility, and Literary Activities.

Introduction

In much of the industrialized world, where access to entertainment has become nearly ubiquitous for many individuals, professional actors constitute a rarified population of experts who receive high levels of attention in the popular press and in public discourse. The work of a small number of professional actors reaches a relatively large swathe of the public, and the average person may be exposed to the work of a professional actor far more often than they are to the efforts of most other kinds of experts (e.g., medical or legal professionals). However, despite the fact that it is possible for actors to reach high-levels of wealth and notoriety, the vast majority of professional actors—even those who work consistently—make a modest wage and have a paycheck-to-paycheck lifestyle more akin to blue-collared workers, rather than other professionals [1]. In addition, most actors experience economic uncertainty throughout their careers, with large temporal gaps in their employment, which can occur regardless of their past success [2]. In addition, the prospect of achieving professional success for undergraduate-level student actors is also far worse than that of most other undergraduate majors (e.g., biology, engineering), with the majority of acting majors leaving the profession within a few years of college graduation [3].

Given this current characterization of the acting profession, with its extremely high-risk features, the question becomes pertinent: what psychological attributes distinguish those individuals who have dedicated themselves to the acting profession from those that have not? And when professional and student actors are jointly considered, what psychological attributes may differentiate those groups, and potentially contribute to professionals’ persistence and success? By closely examining and working to understand the psychological attributes that support actors in their professional work, or their development of expertise as a student, it is our intention to present findings that are not only interesting for the psychological research community but also for professional and student actors themselves. In addition, educators who seek to train student actors who may one day become professional may benefit from such an investigation, because it intends to highlight the dimensions on which professional and student actors differ or are similar, possibly informing pedagogical decisions. Finally, organizations that employ, serve, or represent actors (e.g., theater companies; talent agencies; labor unions) may find value in such an investigation, because it would delineate the strengths and further needs of actors and the acting community.

In order to best accomplish these general aims, we here analyze psychometric data from three groups of individuals: non-acting adults, undergraduate-level student actors, and professional actors who have already achieved a recognized degree of success. From each of these three groups, creative, motivational, and personality attributes are measured, and machine learning methods are utilized to identify psychologically-relevant patterns in these data. But before presenting the current study, we first overview the existing knowledge concerning the psychology of professional actors and highlight areas in which these existing studies can be augmented to provide a richer, and more modern, understanding.

Summary of extant empirical psychological work with professional actors

Actors and acting have been of interest to psychologists since the nascent beginnings of the field, with major early psychologists such as Binet [4] and Vygotsky [5], writing theoretical pieces on the psychology of the performing arts (Binet even co-wrote and produced several popular French plays [6]). Despite this historical interest among psychologists in actors and acting theoretically, the actual body of empirical work dedicated to actors is small compared to investigations into other areas of expertise (e.g., engineering; [7]). The earliest empirical investigation of the psychology of professional actors of which we are aware was conducted by Stacey and Goldberg [8]), who used a number of self-report psychometric scales to show that undergraduate student actors who were regularly cast in university productions were more psychologically similar to professional actors than were undergraduate student actors who were rarely cast in university productions. Specifically, Stacey and Goldberg [8] showed that student actors who were rarely cast exhibited much higher levels of extraversion than did students who were regularly cast or professional actors, who were much more inhibited, conscientious, and prone to depressive thinking.

This early finding paved the way for the continuing investigation of negative psychological attributes in performing artists: a program of research that has been particularly productive in recent years. For example, Dufner and colleagues [9] found that, among dedicated (but not professional) improvisational actors, heightened levels of a narcissistic need for admiration can be observed. However, the improvisational actors showed much lower levels of a narcissistic need to derogate others (known as rivalry), demonstrating that actors may be narcissistically motivated towards self-promotion, but that tendency did not extend to putting other actors down. In related qualitative work, Robb, Due, and Venning [10] conducted in-depth interviews of self-identified professional actors concerning their well-being and vulnerability to mental illness (e.g., anxiety and depressive disorders), finding that professional actors used a wide-range of strategies to protect their well-being including positive engagement with the artistic community, focusing on personal growth, and conceptualizing acting as a meaningful life-purpose. Davison and Furnham [11] used a large sample of professional actors with strict inclusion criteria (i.e., actors were recruited through their agents, assuring they were indeed professional) and administered a number of well-validated self-report measures that were aligned with personality disorder profiles (e.g., Schizoid, Dependent, Obsessive-Compulsive). In general, they identified heighted subclinical levels of personality disorder related traits in their sample: with actors scoring significantly higher than non-actors on Antisocial, Narcissistic, Histrionic, Borderline, and Obsessive-Compulsive personality disorder scales, and only male actors displaying significantly heightened levels of Schizotypal, Avoidant, and Dependent personality disorders. This finding appears to be related to Thomson and Jaque’s [11] finding that professional performing artists (including actors and dancers) who reported significantly greater amounts of Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACEs) also reported experiencing creative states (e.g., a transformational sense of self) more often than performing artists who did not experience as many ACEs.

Using a relatively large sample of self-identified professional actors (82% were members of an actor’s union), Nettle [12] administered questionnaires designed to tap the Big 5 personality dimensions (i.e., Neuroticism, Extraversion, Openness, Conscientiousness, Agreeableness [13]) as well as psychological attributes related to autism spectrum disorder (i.e., Empathizing Quotient and Systemizing Quotient; [14]). Nettle [12] compared actors’ scores on these measures to general population British norms and found that actors scored significantly higher than the normative level on Extraversion, Openness to Experience, and Agreeableness, and had heightened but non-significantly different levels of Neuroticism. The actors also scored significantly higher than the comparison norms on the Empathizing Quotient but were similar to the British norm on the Systemizing Quotient. This general picture of the psychology of professional actors has also led to the use of actors as participants in the investigation of a number of other psychological processes and conditions such as facial recognition of emotions [15] post-traumatic stress disorder [16], and neurological effects of auditory-motor expertise [17]. In addition, some psychological phenomena that specifically affect actors—such as stage fright—have been examined using professional actors as participants [18]. Specifically, these researchers found that, among professional actors, females with low emotional stability and an external locus of control were most at risk for serious and recurring stage fright.

Another recent study specifically of student actors [19] examined the emotional attributes of undergraduate acting majors as compared to undergraduate students without acting experience. These researchers found that actors reported higher temperamental sadness and fear, but more positive viewpoints related to the experience of these negative emotions. In addition, student actors were more capable than other undergraduates at identifying facial expressions related to pride, but less capable than other undergraduates at identifying facial expression related to anger. This finding is related to another recent piece from Ivcevic and colleagues [20] who found that, despite the strong negative correlation at the population level between psychological vulnerabilities such as anxiety and depression and psychological resources such as self-acceptance and hope, creative experts (i.e., fine arts faculty) exhibited simultaneously high levels of both psychological vulnerabilities and resources, implying that creative experts may be fruitfully utilizing both their negative and positive psychological attributes to support their artistic expression. These findings are supported by a relatively long line of psychological research from scholars such as Thalia Goldstein and Ellen Winner [21–24] who have shown that arts education, and specifically training in acting techniques, can support children’s development of emotional regulation, theory of mind, and other positive psychological attributes, including the capacity to safely express negative emotions. In our view, these perennial findings from the developmental and educational literature concerning the benefits of acting training for children further imply that expert of professional actors may not only benefit from such positive psychological attributes that they develop during their training but may actually require those attributes for success in their expert work.

These inferences regarding the expertise of actors are highly related to a line of acting research situated within the literature on expertise development. Noice & Noice [25–27] conducted a series of studies on the cognitive processes that actors use when preparing for their roles. This line of research eventually culminated in Noice & Noice [28, 29] positing a two-stage model of actors’ process: analysis and active experiencing. For Noice and Noice, each of these major stages of the acting process were further broken down into a number of component processes that were often reminiscent of the information-processing perspective on expertise development [30]. For example, in Noice and Noice’s model, finely-grained sub-processes such as causal attribution (when actors define ‘why’ something occurs in a script) can occur many times within the analysis phase of role preparation, depending on the demands of the project and expertise of the actor. In a related but much more recent line of work [31], psychologists have also begun to examine the specific vocal strategies that actors use to embody characters with differing personality traits (e.g., Assertiveness, Cooperativeness), finding that actors altered their voices along 12 vocal parameters (e.g., pitch, volume) in order to portray the personality of their characters.

Promising areas to extend past work

Given the extant understanding of the psychology of professional actors that is present in the field, a number of opportunities to extend, improve, and also replicate existing work are apparent. Here, we briefly delineate areas in which the current state-of-the-art in research on professional actors can be moved forward.

Divergent thinking assessment

One clear pattern that is discernible in the current literature is that there has been a heavy reliance on self-report measures in research on professional actors. This choice is understandable given time and resource limitations in this area of work, but the greater inclusion of performance measures in this line of research remains a strong opportunity. In the larger literature on creativity and creative expertise, the most commonly administered performance measures are Divergent Thinking (DT) assessments, which require participants to generate multiple possible solutions to a given task within a set amount of time [32]. These tasks are typically scored along multiple dimensions that generally correspond to the quantity (i.e. Ideational Fluency) and quality (i.e. Originality) of the ideas generated by a participant, with each of these dimensions having been repeatedly demonstrated to be strong positive predictors of creative potential and performance (see [33] for a review of DT scoring). Given the obvious demand of the acting profession to generate interesting or original ideas rapidly (such as when rehearsing a new role), DT can readily be hypothesized as relevant to the success of professional actors. However, the predictive power of DT to identify those individuals who are or could be professional actors remains unknown in the field. As far as we are aware, DT assessments have not yet been systematically administered to professional actors as part of research study, however, some initial evidence that DT measures are sensitive to acting training is available in the field. For example, Sowden and colleagues [34] demonstrated that improvisation exercises could improve the DT of elementary school students, suggesting that DT measures may be suitable for identifying individuals with acting training. In this investigation, performance assessment of DT is included, as a key way to extend past work.

Richer array of self-report questionnaires

Although many psychologically interesting and well-validated self-report scales have been previously included in research on actors, many relevant constructs remain to be included. For example, self-reported creative activities in domains in which an individual is not a professional (e.g., creative visual arts activities for actors) have previously been shown to be predictive in creativity research [35]. For instance, it may be reasonable to hypothesize that actors, given the creative nature of their work, will engage in more creative activities than non-actors even in domains (e.g., literature; music) that are not directly within the area of acting. Relatedly, it could also be, that because actors’ work demands creative thinking, they may tend to avoid expending creative effort in other more quotidian domains such as cooking.

In addition, motivational constructs such as Grit [36] that have come to greater attention in the literature recently, have never been examined with professional actors before. In the context of the acting profession, where financial security can be lacking, and rejection (i.e., not booking an auditioned-for role) is commonplace, motivational attributes such as perseverance in the face of adversity and consistency of interest in one’s chosen profession—two principal facets of Grit [36]—appear likely to be relevant. Emotional Intelligence [37] also appears to be a candidate for relevance to professional actors, in that actors’ work regularly consists of appraising and using emotions. Finally, although the Big 5 personality attributes have previously been investigated in professional actors [12], the more finely-grained analysis of personality facets (two of which load on each of the Big 5 [38]) has never before been examined with actors. More specifically, the Big 5 dimensions of Neuroticism, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, Extraversion, and Openness can be further delineated into 10 facets: Neuroticism contains both Volatility and Withdrawal; Agreeableness is divided into both Compassion and Politeness; Conscientiousness contains Industriousness and Orderliness; Extraversion has Enthusiasm and Assertiveness; and Openness is divided into Intellect and Openness. In this study, each of these extensions to past work are included.

Inclusion of student and professional actors

Many scholars who have studied actors and acting have been interested with the process by which student or trainee actors become professionals and subsequently develop expertise [29, 31]. However, only rarely in the literature have student actors and more expert professional actors been specifically compared on their psychological attributes. In addition, within the general literature on creativity, psychological differences among individuals who are professionally creative and those who are not has perennially been of interest [39]. Regarding the specific question of how student and professional actors differ psychologically, the classic study—now nearly 70 years old—by Stacey and Goldberg [8] remains potentially the most informative. In the current study, we adopt a sampling strategy similar to Stacey and Goldberg [8] in that we specifically compare student and professional actors but modernize the work through the large-scale data collection that is now the standard of the field [40].

Modern machine learning methodology

Some existing studies of professional actors (e.g., [12, 40]) have collected impressively large samples given the challenges in recruiting this population for psychological research. However, within this literature, sample sizes have not necessarily been fully leveraged through the application of cutting-edge methodologies. Indeed, nearly all quantitative work in the study of professional actors have solely utilized traditional mean-comparison methods (e.g., ANOVA) or ordinary-least-square predictive methods (e.g., regression). Although these perennially applied methods are not inherently flawed, the opportunity to methodologically modernize the methods used to investigate the psychology of professional actors seems apparent. Specifically, with more psychological research incorporating advanced methodologies from the machine-learning family [41] opportunities to potentially illuminate more nuanced, more generalizable, and possibly more replicable (i.e., not over-fit to a single dataset) patterns in our data become apparent. In this study, we used machine-learning methodology in two different ways. First, the DT assessment is scored using a modern computational scoring method derived from the text-mining literature [42], and Least Absolute Shrinkage and Selection Operators (LASSOs [43]) are utilized to provide the most psychologically informative prediction and classification models.

Research questions of current study

Given the current state-of-the-literature reviewed here concerning empirical psychological research with professional actors, as well as the previously identified areas to extend that extant work, the current study posits three specific research questions to be investigated:

How do non-actors, student actors, and professional actors differ on average on a number of theoretically-relevant psychological attributes?

Can actors be effectively distinguished from non-actors based on their psychological attributes? What psychological attributes would be strongly-weighted by a model used for this purpose?

Can professional actors be effectively distinguished from undergraduate student actors based on their psychological attributes? What psychological attributes would be strongly-weighted by a model used for this purpose?

Methodology

To facilitate replicability and open science, the data collected and analyzed in this study is archived at Zenodo (LINK: https://zenodo.org/record/3899579#.X3-M81KSlaQ; DOI: 10.5281/zenodo.3899578), and the text-mining based models used to score the divergent thinking tasks are freely available on our laboratory website (https://openscoring.du.edu/).

Participants

A total of 296 individuals participated in this study, with participants being drawn from three different groups: (a) non-acting adults, (b) undergraduate students majoring in acting, and (c) adult professional actors. Each of these groups of participants are described separately in this section. All participant recruitment strategies used here, as well as study procedures, were approved by the Institutional Review Board at the University of Denver.

Non-acting adults

This group included 92 (53 female; 57.6%) participants. Non-acting adults were recruited for this study via Amazon Mechanical Turk, a crowdsourcing platform widely used in psychology research, including creativity research [44]. Because of the high language demands of divergent thinking tasks, participants were required to report themselves as fluent English speakers in order to participate, although 2 participants (2.1%) reported English as their second (but fluent) language. Participants were compensated $3.00 each for their participation. Participants were required to be over the age of 18 to participate, but the minimum actual participant age was 21, with a maximum age of 68. The mean age of participants was 37 (SD = 10.58). The majority of participants (n = 68; 73.91%) reported their race/ethnicity as White or European-American, while smaller proportions of the sample reported their ethnicity as Black or African-American (n = 6; 6.5%), Asian (n = 9; 9.8%), Latinx (n = 5; 5.43) or multiple ethnicities (n = 4; 4.2%).

Although this sample was collected as a non-acting comparison group, we did not require that these participants should have had zero history of activities within the performing arts. Indeed, some history of “little-c” [45] creative activities is likely to be expected of nearly any sample. However, as will be presented in the Results section of this paper (see Table 2 for standardized descriptive statistics), this comparison group did report statistically and practically significantly fewer creative activities than the other groups for every creative domain measured, with the greatest differences being within the performing arts domain.

Table 2

Means, standard deviations, and univariate mean comparisons.
VariableNon-Acting AdultsUndergraduate Acting MajorsProfessional ActorsOne-way Mean Comparison
Fluency-.38 (.78).07 (.94).27 (1.11)F(2,293) = 11.99, p .001, η 2 = .08
Elaboration-.28 (.77).05 (1.02).20 (1.09)F(2,293) = 5.94, p = .003, η 2 = .03
Originality (mean).06 (.93)-.29 (1.22).23 (.71)F(2,293) = 7.39, p .001, η 2 = .05
Originality (max)-.19 (.95)-.04 (1.02).21 (.98)F(2,293) = 4.33, p = .014, η 2 = .03
Literary Activities-0.81 (.91).23 (.81).50 (.78)F(2,293) = 66.41, p .001, η 2 = .31
Musical Activities-.86 (.79).38 (.81).39 (.83)F(2,293) = 74.50, p .001, η 2 = .33
Crafting Activities-.69 (.99).32 (.85).30 (.83)F(2,293) = 40.96, p .001, η 2 = .22
Cooking Activities-.33 (1.00).35 (.94)-.05 (.95)F(2,293) = 12.64, p .001, η 2 = .08
Visual Art Activities-.57 (.91).31 (.85).20 (.99)F(2,293) = 25.59, p .001, η 2 = .22
Performing Arts Activities-.92 (.61).42 (.85).40 (.85)F(2,293) = 91.45, p .001, η 2 = .38
Grit-.32 (1.15).04 (.92).25 (.84)F(2,293) = 7.91, p .001, η 2 = .05
Intolerance of Uncertainty (Prospective).40 (1.01)-.35 (.90)-.02 (.95)F(2,293) = 14.70, p .001, η 2 = .09
Intolerance of Uncertainty (Inhibitory).24 (1.09)-.31 (.88).09 (.94)F(2,293) = 8.60, p .001, η 2 = .05
Self-emotional Appraisal.02 (.97).18 (.86)-.19 (1.11)F(2,293) = 3.71, p = .03, η 2 = .02
Other-emotional Appraisal-.39 (1.14).32 (.69).03 (.99)F(2,293) = 13.54, p .001, η 2 = .08
Uses of Emotion.11 (1.02).08 (.89)-.17 (1.05)F(2,293) = 2.55, p = .08, η 2 = .02
Openness-.73 (1.20).32 (.71).34 (.62)F(2,293) = 35.95, p .001, η 2 = .24
Intellect-.23 (1.12).22 (1.02)-.03 (.81)F(2,293) = 4.99, p = .007, η 2 = .03
Enthusiasm-.47 (1.10).24 (.85).19 (.89)F(2,293) = 17.11, p .001, η 2 = .10
Assertiveness-.59 (1.21).31 (.71).23 (.78)F(2,293) = 28.17, p .001, η 2 = .16
Industriousness.42 (1.06)-.17 (.85)-.21 (.96)F(2,293) = 13.19, p .001, η 2 = .08
Orderliness.10 (1.21)-.14 (.84).05 (.92)F(2,293) = 1.67, p = .191, η 2 = .01
Compassion-.42 (1.35).29 (.67).09 (.75)F(2,293) = 14.37, p .001, η 2 = .08
Politeness.07 (1.15)-.06 (.90)-.02 (.94)F(2,293) = 0.52, p = .593, η 2 < .01
Volatility-.24 (1.21)-.03 (.91).23 (.84)F(2,293) = 5.57, p = .003, η 2 = .04
Withdrawal-.24 (1.20)-.02 (.88).22 (.86)F(2,293) = 5.72, p = .004, η 2 = .04

Note: Groups means are presented in cells, SD’s are in parentheses. All variables measured here are standardized across the entire sample: the grand mean is zero and grand SD is 1. But, means and SD’s can differ across the groups, and therefore the comparisons here are relevant. Bolded variable names indicate that variable exhibited significant differences across groups at the p .05 level.

Undergraduate acting majors

100 undergraduate students, currently enrolled as acting or theater majors, participated in this study. Recruitment for this group of participants was primarily accomplished via existing social media listservs that connected undergraduate theater and acting students, although snowball sampling methods, in which participating students shared the participation opportunity with their classmates, were also utilized. The average age of the undergraduate students was 20.33 (SD = 2.65), with a minimum age of 18 and a maximum age of 26. The sample was relatively evenly split among students in terms of the years they had spent in their undergraduate program: of the 87 student actors who reported their year-in-program, 24 (27.59%) were in their first year, 15 (17.24%) were in their second year, 18 (20.69%) were in their third year, 19 (21.84%) were in their fourth year, and 11 (12.64) were in their fifth year as undergraduates. In addition, although all undergraduate actors had received targeted acting training as part of their education, they were situated in three different concentrations within their programs: of the 78 student actors who reported their concentrations 35 (44.87) were in an acting concentration, 27 (34.61%) were in musical theater, and 16 (20.51%) were in a directing, playwriting, or production concentration. 77 (77.00%) students reported a female gender, 18 (18.00%) reported male, and 5 (5.00%) reported a non-binary gender identity. 77 (77.00%) reported an ethnicity of White or European-American, 6 (6.00%) reported their ethnicity as Black or African-American, 2 (2.00%) Asian, 4 (4.00%) Latinx, and 11 (11.00%) multiple ethnicities. 95% (n = 95) reported being a native English speaker, with 5% (n = 5) reporting a first language other than English.

Professional actors

In this study, 104 professional actors were recruited via existing email and social media listservs that connected members of professional actor’s unions: either Actors Equity (which focuses on the representation of stage actors) or SAG-AFTRA (which focuses on the representation of screen actors). During the ongoing data collection process, some snowball sampling of professional actors occurred, where individual actors would share the participation opportunity with their colleagues. In order to be eligible to participate, actors needed to report being a member of either Actor’s Equity of SAG-AFTRA, or if they were not union-affiliated, report having previously booked more than 10 professional acting contracts either on stage or screen. In addition, 56.38% (n = 53) reported holding a Bachelor’s degree in acting, while 29.79% (n = 28) reported holding a Master’s degree, and 3.19% (n = 3) reported holding a doctoral degree. 5 participants (5.32%) reported having no university training, and the same proportion reported having a non-degree certification from a university. 72.34% (n = 68) also reported having engaged in additional acting training at a studio apart from their university training.

In terms of gender, 51.92% (n = 54) of the professional actors reported female, 45.19% (n = 47) reported male, .96% (n = 1) reported a non-binary gender, and 1.92% (n = 2) preferred not to respond. The average age of the sample was 35.43 (SD = 10.13) with minimum age of 21 and a maximum age of 67. The majority of participants (n = 84; 70.77%) reported their race/ethnicity as White or European-American, while smaller proportions of the sample reported their ethnicity as Black or African-American (n = 3; 2.88%), Asian (n = 1; .96%), Latinx (n = 6; 5.77%) or multiple ethnicities (n = 6; 5.77%). 97.12% (n = 101) of the professional actors reported they were native English speakers, with 2.88% (n = 3) reporting a first language other than English.

Measures

In order to capture a variety of meaningful psychological attributes that may explain individuals’ continued motivation and ability to engage with the acting profession, a number of well-validated measures were administered to participants. All scales administered here were scored using confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) models and empirical Bayes, in order to most validly quantify the psychological attribute tapped by the measure [46]. As such, three reliability indices are available in Table 1 for each of the measures: Cronbach’s alpha [47], McDonald’s Omega [48], and Hancock’s H [49]. These reliability indices differ in that alpha assumes a tau-equivalent model (i.e. all item loadings equal) and therefore represents a lower-bound of the scale reliability [50]. Omega allows for assumption of tau-equivalence to be relaxed (i.e., item loadings can differ), and also accounts for the size of the error terms in a factor model [51] H is also referred to as maximal reliability [52], because it provides an upper-bound on the reliability of the scale and is most appropriate when measure scores are saved directly from a latent measurement model (as is the case in this study). Details about the measures appear below.

Table 1

Reliability indices for measures used in this investigation.
Measure / DimensionReliability
αωH
Alternate Uses Task
Fluency.962.964.965
Elaboration.966.967.968
Originality (mean).858.874.903
Originality (max).824.834.842
Inventory of Creative Activities
Literature.797.804.821
Music.876.881.945
Crafts.873.882.953
Cooking.872.879.899
Visual Art.817.826.847
Performing Arts.831.836.902
Short Grit Scale
Grit.807.811.863
Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale
Prospective Uncertainty.866.868.871
Inhibitory Uncertainty.877.878.887
Emotional Intelligence Scale
Self-emotion Appraisal.886.894.909
Other-emotion Appraisal.913.917.940
Uses of Emotion.864.874.903
Big 5 Aspects Scale
Openness.876.882.893
Intellect.846.855.892
Enthusiasm.924.927.933
Assertiveness.931.935.946
Industriousness.879.886.891
Orderliness.889.894.907
Compassion.952.953.957
Politeness.827.832.852
Volatility.941.942.945
Withdrawal.920.921.924

Alternate Uses Task (AUT)

The AUT is a psychometric measure in which participants are asked to generate as many creative uses for an object as possible within a certain amount of time (i.e., two minutes per object in this case). The AUT has been used for assessing divergent thinking and creative potential for decades [53–55], and remains one of the most-often utilized tasks within the creativity research literature [56, 57]. The following 10 object names were presented to participants in a randomized order: book, fork, table, hammer, pants, bottle, brick, tire, shovel, and shoe. The resulting AUT data are both open-ended (i.e., participants can differ on how many responses they give within the time limit) and ill-defined (i.e., participants can also differ on the number of words used to describe their responses), and therefore the AUT has typically been scored using a number of different scoring procedures, each designed to estimate a different dimension of divergent thinking [58, 59]. In this study, three scoring procedures were utilized for the AUT, and each are described below.

Ideational Fluency. Ideational Fluency refers to an individual’s capacity to rapidly generate a number of ideas within a set amount of time [58]. Therefore, the scoring procedure for Fluency is relatively simple: the number of responses generated by each participant for each AUT item was tallied. These ten item-level Ideational Fluency scores exhibited a high level of composite reliability (see Table 1 for alpha), and when a unidimensional confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) model was fit to those item-level Fluency counts, they also showed a high level of factor reliability (see Table 1 for Omega and H). Fluency scores for each participant in the dataset were estimated via the CFA model using empirical Bayes, and saved for later analysis.

Elaboration. Elaboration refers to the degree that participants explicate their responses to the AUT [60] and within the creativity research literature Elaboration is commonly scored using word counts [61]. Here, the number of words participants utilized within each AUT item was counted, producing ten item-level Elaboration scores. These scores displayed a strong level of reliability both at the composite and latent factor levels and participants’ Elaboration scores for the AUT were generated from a unidimensional CFA model using empirical Bayes.

Originality. Typically regarded as the most theoretically important dimension of divergent thinking measured by the AUT, Originality refers to the relative unusualness or novelty of participant responses [62]. A number of different scoring procedures for Originality exist in the creativity research literature, and this study follows the most modern methodological guidelines available [33]. Specifically, Originality is scored here via a text-mining approach, using the Global Vectors for Word Representation (GloVe) 840B system, which is publicly available through the Stanford natural-language-processing laboratory [63]. This text-mining model was trained on a corpus of 840 billion words that were scraped from a variety of online sources including Wikipedia and Twitter. Previous psychometric work in creativity research [42] showed that, among a number of candidate text-mining models, GloVe was the most capable of approximating human-rated Originality, displayed the most advantageous reliability coefficients, and had the most theoretically meaningful correlations to relevant criteria measures. In addition, in accordance with current methodological recommendations within creativity research [61] an inverse-document-frequency (IDF) weighting scheme was applied to the GloVe scoring system, in which the words utilized in participant responses were weighted more strongly if they were rare in model’s training corpus and weighted more weakly if they were common in the model’s training corpus [64].

Conceptually, GloVe is designed to preserve the linearity of the relations among words, so that the semantic distances between them are directly comparable by studying the factorization matrix of words by latent dimensions. Using these matrices, the latent dimensions can be used as coordinates in a geometrically represented space, and the cosine of the angle between the word-vectors can be interpreted as the semantic or associative distance among words [65]. As an example with the AUT, if the prompt was “shovel”, the response “dig a hole” would result in a vector that has an acute angle with the vector for “shovel”. In contrast, the response “fling tennis balls for a dog to chase” would result in a vector that has a wider angle from the initial prompt vector, indicating that response is less semantically similar. Please see Fig 1 for a visualization of the specific geometric relations among these example responses. The cosines of these angles among word vectors yielded semantic similarity indices that ranges from -1 to 1 for each AUT response, which were subtracted from 1 in order to yield Originality scores that ranged from 0 to 2. Originality scores were then averaged across the responses generated for each AUT item (e.g., Book), resulting in ten item-level Originality scores for each participant. These scores displayed a satisfactory level of reliability at both the composite-scale and latent factor levels and Originality scores were saved for each participant from the unidimensional CFA model using empirical Bayes.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc. Object name is pone.0240728.g001.jpg

A visual representation of the geometric relations among response vectors arising from a GloVe 840B scoring analysis.

As can be seen, the more original a response, the greater its angle with the AUT prompt.

Inventory of creative activities

This inventory is a relatively recently developed [35] self-report measure for real-life creative activities and accomplishments across eight domains: literature, music, arts and crafts, cooking, sports, visual arts, performing arts, and science and engineering. Given the general nature of this sample, and time-constraints on the data collection, we administered the scales for six of those original eight domains: music, literature, arts and crafts, cooking, visual arts, and performing arts. Each of these scales consisted of six Likert-style items that ask participants how many times they have done particular creative activities in the past 10 years with five response categories: never, 1–2 times, 3–5 times, 6–10 times, and more than 10 times. For example, in the music domain, participants are asked how many times they have written a piece of music, or created a mix tape, among other items. In the arts and crafts domain, participants are asked how many times they created an original decoration. In cooking, how many times they made up a new recipe. The visual and performing arts scale asks how many times participants painted a picture and performed in a play, respectively. All of these scales exhibited satisfactory reliability, with the Music Activities scale having the highest reliability, and the Literary Activities scale having the lowest reliability.

Grit scale

This 12-item self-report measure aims to tap participants’ levels of a motivational construct termed grit that includes both the consistency of participants’ interest in and their perseverance on their long-term goals [66]. In previous work, Grit has been shown to predict educational and career outcomes across a variety of domains [36]. In its original conceptualization, this short measure contained two sub-scales: Consistency of Interest (e.g., I often set a goal but later choose to pursue a different one [reverse coded]) and Perseverance of Effort (e.g., I finish whatever I begin). Items on the Consistency of Interest sub-scale are all reverse-coded, while items on Perseverance of Effort scale are not. In this investigation, the individual sub-scales of this measure did not reach adequate reliability, so following with existing practice in the literature [67], the Grit Scale was scored unidimensionally, with all 12-items indicating a single underlying construct of Grit. Unidimensionally, the Grit Scale reached satisfactory reliability at both the composite (Cronbach’s alpha) and factor (Omega, H) levels.

Intolerance of uncertainty scale

This 12-item scale is designed to tap participants psychological need for certainty, or their fear of the uncertain or unknown [68]. Intolerance of Uncertainty has been a key construct in current psychological understandings of worry, a phenomenon that is related to career choice [69]. This measure features two sub-scales: Prospective Anxiety (e.g., I always want to know what the future has in store for me), which is designed to tap how participants react to the possibility of an uncertain future, and Inhibitory Anxiety (e.g., The smallest doubt can stop me from acting), which focuses on the effect of uncertainty on participant behavior in the present. In this study, both of these scales exhibited satisfactory reliability and were scored separately, creating two scale-level scores for analysis.

Emotional intelligence scale

Broadly conceptualized as the ability to perceive, understand, and use emotions in oneself and in others [37], emotional intelligence has been measured in many ways in past literature, including performance tests [70] and self-report. Here, a short 12-item self-reported Emotional Intelligence measure [71] was administered. This measure features three scales: Self-emotional Appraisal (e.g., I have a good understanding of my own emotions), Others’ emotional appraisal (e.g., I am sensitive to the feelings and emotions of others), and Uses of emotion (e.g., I always encourage myself to try my best). Each of these scales reached satisfactory reliability and were scores separately, creating 3 scale scores for future analysis.

Big five aspects scale

The Big Five Aspects Scale (BFAS; [38]) is a widely utilized self-report personality measure in which participants indicate levels of five principal aspects of personality, each of which is divided further into two facets. The “big five” dimensions of personality—Neuroticism, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, Extraversion, and Openness—are all available on this measure. However, each of these principal five personality characteristics are further divided into two facets: Neuroticism contains both Volatility (e.g., I get upset easily) and Withdrawal (e.g., I am afraid of many things); Agreeableness contains Compassion (e.g., I like to do things for others) and Politeness (e.g., I hate to seem pushy); Conscientiousness is divided into both Industriousness (e.g., I am not easily distracted) and Orderliness (e.g., I want everything to be ‘just right’); Extraversion has Enthusiasm (e.g., I have a lot of fun) and Assertiveness (e.g., I know how to captivate people); and Openness is divided in to Intellect (e.g., I like to solve complex problems) and Openness (e.g., I need a creative outlet). In this investigation each of the ten facets displayed strong reliability, and scoring therefore occurred at the facet-level, creating ten scores for later analysis.

Procedures

All participation in this study was conducted via the Internet with Qualtrics administration software. Informed consent was obtained before participants could move forward with the measures (these procedures were approved by the Institutional Review Board at the University of Denver). Study instructions asked participants to complete the measures with minimal distractions and recommended that they turn off electronic devices as well as close other websites or programs open on their computer. Because the AUT requires a significant amount of typing, participation required a traditional keyboard and participation via smartphone or tablet was not allowed. Participants were given two minutes to provide uses for each AUT item before they were automatically advanced to the next object, and they could not advance before those two minutes were up. After responding to all ten objects (i.e., after 20 minutes), participants were informed that the task was complete, and moved to the self-report portion of the study. After all self-report measures were complete, participants responded to the demographic questions and logged out of the study website.

Results and implications

Analysis of these data unfolded in two major phases. First, average levels of each of the constructs included in this study across the non-acting, student actor, and professional actor groups, in addition to significance tests for mean differences among those groups, are presented. Then, to provide a holistic picture of how these psychological attributes can, in concert, be used to describe the general attributes of actors, and even identify professional from student actors, a sequence of two Least Absolute Shrinkage and Selection Operator (LASSO; [72]) models are fit. These models are used to first identify those participants who are actors (either students or professionals) out of the entire sample, and then to further sort professional from student actors, when the comparison sample is left out of the analysis.

Descriptive statistics and univariate mean comparisons

The measurement procedures used here produced 26 psychologically-relevant scores for each of the 296 participants in the dataset. These scores were produced via a CFA model on a standardized (z-score) metric, meaning that the grand mean across all participants was zero and standard deviation was one. However, means for the specific groups (i.e., non-actors, student actors, and professional actors) did exhibit statistically and practically significant differences. Please see Table 2 for means and standard deviations per group, as well as accompanying significance tests and effect sizes. At the p < .05 significance level, only three measurements did not exhibit significant mean differences (and the smallest effect-sizes): Politeness, Orderliness, and Uses of Emotion. Even at the much more conservative, Scheffé corrected significance level of .05÷26 = .0019, 17 out of the 26 constructs would still exhibit significant differences. In the sections that follow, each of these differences are presented, and immediate implications are briefly highlighted.

Divergent thinking

Fluency, Elaboration, and maximal Originality scoring of the AUT all showed significant differences among the groups in the following way: non-actors scored the lowest (below the grand mean), student actors scored in the middle (around the grand mean) and professional actors scored the highest (above the grand mean). However, the mean Originality scoring showed significant differences in a different pattern, in which the student actors scored the lowest, non-actors in the middle, and professional actors highest. All four of these measurements contribute to the general finding that professional actors have an average divergent thinking ability level that is heightened above non-actors and student actors, at least on a verbal task like the AUT. Of the dimensions of divergent thinking measured here, the strongest univariate effect size was found with Fluency.

Creative activities

The strongest effect-sizes present in these mean comparisons were found on the Inventory of Creative Activities: with the Performing Arts scale having the largest effect-size, followed by Music and Literary Activities. In addition, these three scales differed among the groups in a predictable way, in that the non-actors scored substantially below the grand mean on average, while both the student and professional actors scored above the grand mean on average. Interestingly, the student actors reported slightly more performing arts activities than the professional actors, perhaps because many opportunities for such activities exist in the college setting, and this scale did not differentiate among paid and un-paid activities. The cooking activities scale, somewhat counter to stereotype, indicated that undergraduate actors were engaged in substantially more cooking activities than were non-actors or professional actors.

Grit

Following the pattern of much of the Inventory of Creative Activities and the AUT, the Grit Scale showed significant differences across groups with non-actors having the lowest, students being in the middle, and professional actors having the most Grit. Given the highly competitive nature of the acting profession, this finding may indicate that Grit supports professional actors as they transition from student to professional life.

Intolerance of uncertainty

In contrast to this pattern, the Intolerance of Uncertainty scale showed the highest average scores for the non-actors (who were above the grand mean), indicating that they had the greatest need for certainty or fear of the unknown, while professional actors scored in the middle (around the grand mean), and student actors reported very low levels of Intolerance of Uncertainty (below the grand mean), or, worded a different way, high levels of tolerance of uncertainty. In line with previous findings about the difficulties faced by professional actors in their uncertain job market [1], this finding implies that professional actors, despite having a lower Intolerance of Uncertainty than non-acting adults, do not exhibit the very low levels of Intolerance of Uncertainty that student actors report. This pattern, in which student actors exhibit less Intolerance of Uncertainty than do professional actors, is perhaps not surprising given that they are situated within university contexts that are potentially more likely than the professional context to be personally supportive of individuals, and they are also much less likely than professional actors to be solely responsible for their livelihood (e.g., student actors may rely on financial aid or assistance from caregivers).

Emotional intelligence

In a pattern that potentially runs counter to what would be expected given the emotional nature of Actors’ work, professional actors reported an average level of Self-emotional Appraisal (a dimension of Emotional Intelligence) that was below the grand mean, non-actors reported an amount of Self-emotional Appraisal around the grand mean, and student actors reported substantially more than the grand mean. In contrast, professional actors reported an average level of Other-emotional appraisal that was around the grand mean, with student actors reporting substantially more than the grand mean and non-actors reporting substantially less. Taken together, these findings imply that actors’ Emotional Intelligence (at least when self-reported) appears to be focused at others and not at themselves, although student actors did report heightened levels of both these facets of Emotional Intelligence, complicating that picture. Another potential explanation of this finding is that professional actors may be better calibrated in terms of their true levels of Emotional Intelligence, whereas non-actors and students may have exhibited a self-report bias that led them to over-estimate their true levels of Emotional Intelligence, therefore moving their group means above the professional actors’ group mean.

Big five personality facets

As one of the only constructs included here that has been investigated previously within the literature on professional actors [12], the Big Five Personality traits are here examined with a high degree of specificity in that each of the Big 5 dimensions were quantified as two sub-dimensions or facets. In some cases (e.g., Extraversion), significant differences on one facet were in the opposite direction of the other facet, strongly suggesting the importance of facet-level measurement. Patterns of findings are discussed below.

Openness/Intellect. These two facets of personality are without a doubt the most widely studied in research on creativity and creative individuals and are currently considered the core of the creative personality [73]. However, patterns in significant mean differences across the three groups on these two facets were not the same. Specifically, both professional and student actors reported levels of Openness that was substantially above the grand mean, while non-actors reported levels of Openness that was substantially below the grand mean. The effect-size associated with the Openness significance test was the largest across any construct measured in this study, besides Creative Activities. This pattern implies that Openness, as a facet of personality, is a strong discriminator of individuals who are or are not actors but does not differ across professional and student actors. The Intellect facet, on the other hand did differ among the student and professional actors, with student actors reporting much more Intellect than the professionals. In addition, the effect size associated with Intellect was much smaller than that associated with Openness, implying that Openness, more than Intellect, is a critical indicator of the acting personality.

Extraversion. Extraversion has long been associated with acting [8], and creative careers in general [74]. Here, significant group differences across non-actors, student actors, and professional actors were observed on both the Enthusiasm and Assertiveness facets of Extraversion. In both cases, the non-actors reported the lowest amounts (below the grand mean) and both the professional and student actors reported above the grand mean. Also in both facets, the student actors reported slightly more Extraversion than did the professionals, but the difference was more marked in Assertiveness, which displayed a larger effect size than did Enthusiasm.

Conscientiousness. In previous work (e.g., [75]), conscientiousness has been shown to be inversely related to creativity, and this study replicated that finding. Although the Orderliness facet of conscientiousness did not display significant differences, implying that the groups included in this study did not differ statistically on this attribute, the Industriousness facet did show significant differences with the non-actors reporting the greatest average amount of Industriousness (above the grand mean) and both the student and professional acting groups reporting substantially less Industriousness than the grand mean. This finding suggests that previous evidence about Conscientiousness and its inverse relation to creative work may have been driven by the Industriousness facet. As a possibility, the Industriousness facet may be specifically inversely related to mind-wandering activities that have been shown to support creative insight [76].

Agreeableness. Although no significant differences were found on the Politeness facet of Agreeableness, significant differences were found on Compassion. Specifically, non-actors reported the lowest average levels of Compassion (below the grand mean), professional actors reported a level of Compassion that was right around the grand mean, and student actors reported the highest levels of Compassion, substantially higher than the grand mean. This finding suggests that actors in general have heightened levels of Compassion, but that pattern is most marked with student actors, who report more Compassion than do professionals.

Neuroticism. Both the Volatility and Withdrawal facets of Neuroticism displayed significant differences across the groups, and the differences on both those facets were in the same direction. In particular, the non-actors displayed the lowest levels of Neuroticism (below the grand mean), student actors reported levels of both facets of Neuroticism that was around the grand mean, and professional actors reported the highest levels of Neuroticism (above the grand mean). This finding is in line with previous work (e.g. [12]) that observed heightened levels of Neuroticism with professional actors, as well as other classic work (e.g. [8]) that argued that this personality difference was a key to understanding the differences among student and professional actors.

LASSO classification models

Although the univariate mean comparisons presented in the previous section are relevant to understanding the psychology of professional actors, they fall short of providing a full picture of how actors—whether student or professional—holistically differ from non-actors, and how professional actors are distinguished from students psychologically. In effect, the univariate tests are useful in that they each isolate a single psychological construct and elucidate group differences on that construct. But, as has been understood in multivariate psychology for many years (e.g. [77]), univariate comparisons among groups may mask important differences that can emerge when several constructs are analyzed simultaneously. There are a variety of existing analytic strategies to find substantively meaningful patterns in high-dimensional multivariate data like that used here (see [78]). One of those strategies is to reverse the predictive direction of the general linear models used above—where the categorical group indicator was the predictor, and the continuous measurement was the outcome—and attempt to use the 26 psychological attributes measured in this study to effectively sort the participants back into their groups (i.e., continuous attributes will be predictors, categorical groups will be the outcome).

In order to accomplish this conceptual and analytic strategy, there are a number of available statistical methodologies. For example, discriminant function analysis has historically been, and remains useful in psychology to classify participants (e.g., [79]), and logistic regression (as well as its ordinal and multinomial versions) has also been a fruitfully applied model (e.g., [80]). However, these traditional statistical methods do have a number of limitations worth considering here. For one, these methods require an a-priori specified list of predictor variables that enter the model in a specific order: decisions that researchers sometimes do not have the theoretical knowledge to make, and that can have hidden influences on the strength and even direction of the predictive relations from the predictors to the outcome [81]. In addition, these models are frequently over-fit, meaning that their predictive capacity (e.g., R-square) is almost always strongest in the dataset to which they are originally fit, and are weaker in validation or replication datasets; a phenomenon known as shrinkage [82].

A more modern alternative to these methods is LASSO, which estimates a penalization parameter (λ), that is applied to the model parameters in order to minimize its degree of over-fitting and therefore minimize future shrinkage of the coefficients should the model be fit to a different dataset [72, 83]. The larger the λ of a LASSO model, the more penalized (i.e., reduced) the LASSO coefficients will be, relative to a traditional method. In addition, LASSO algorithmically enters the predictor variables into the model in order to determine which predictors are necessary to retain (i.e., are significant predictors), and which predictors can be dropped from the model. LASSO models can also be specifically cross-validated by folding the analytic dataset a certain number of times and estimating a model that minimizes over-fitting and future shrinkage as well as selecting maximally important predictors for classification of participants [43]. In addition, a logistic version of LASSO, in which the outcome variables are categorical, has been meaningfully used in the psychological literature as a classification model (e.g. [84, 85]). Given the advantages of LASSO, it was applied here to test the capacity of the 26 psychological constructs measured in this study to discriminate actors (either students or professionals) from non-actors, and to sort professional and student actors from one another.

In interpreting the results of the LASSO models in this study, and especially in interpreting how the results of the LASSO models differ from the results of the univariate tests above, it is important to consider that the LASSO coefficients are meant to represent the predictive capacity of that variable with all other variables in the model controlled for. The results of the LASSO models below can be substantively different than the univariate results above for this reason. For instance, it is possible (and even likely) that some variables that exhibited univariate differences above will be dropped from the LASSO models due to their lack of predictive power over and above the other included variables. The inverse is also possible: just because a variable did not display univariate differences above does not mean that it is not a useful predictor of acting, when combined with the other variables in the model. For these methodological reasons, we would argue that the LASSO coefficients calculated here are a more true-to-life depiction of how professional and student actors differ from non-actors, or how professional and student actors differ from each other. This is because the various psychological attributes studied here do not, in truth, operate in an isolated or univariate way. Instead, these psychological attributes operate in conjunction with one another in order to support individuals in their creative development and profession.

Identifying actors

Using all 296 participants included in this study, a cross-validated LASSO model was fit using all 26 measured constructs as potential predictors to sort non-actors (n = 92, coded 0) from actors (professional and student combined n = 204, coded 1). The model was cross-validated on ten folds of the dataset. The unpenalized model attained an R-square of .68 (equivalent to maximum likelihood logistic regression), but the optimal λ parameter estimated by the model was .01, and when applied to the model coefficients, reduced the R-square to .63. This LASSO model produced a predicted probability of being an actor for every participant in the dataset. If that predicted probability was over .50, they were considered to have been sorted into the actors’ group, below .50 they were considered to have been sorted into the non-actors’ group. Please see Table 3 for full classification accuracy information on these models. The LASSO model was capable of correctly identifying 96.57% (n = 197) of the actors in these data, correctly identifying 81.53% (n = 75) of the non-actors, and had a total classification accuracy of 91.9%.

Table 3

Classification tables from LASSO models.
Sorting Actors from Non-Actors (Model 1)
True CategorySorted CategoryTotal
Non-ActorActor
Non-Actor751792
Actor797104
Total82214296
Sorting Professional from Student Actors (Model 2)
True CategorySorted CategoryTotal
StudentProfessional
Student6337100
Professional2975104
Total92112204